We propose that there are at least three general classes of psychological mechanism that contribute to political polarization. These mechanisms are best characterized as cognitive–motivational, insofar as they involve the goal-directed (motivational) use of information processing (cognition), as described in several classic social psychological theories88,96,97. Ego-justifying mechanisms involve the self-serving tendency to advance and maintain one’s own pre-existing beliefs, opinions and values and to defend against information that might contradict any of those98,99. Group-justifying mechanisms, which are central to social identity theory88,90,91,94, involve group-serving tendencies to advance and maintain the interests and assumptions of the in-group against one or more out-groups (‘us versus them’). System-justifying mechanisms, which are often overlooked in social psychological analyses of political polarization, reflect the fact that some individuals and groups are motivated to preserve the status quo — and to resist various forms of social change — while others are motivated to challenge or improve upon it100,101. Once groups are ideologically polarized (that is, extreme), their members may engage in ‘normal’ or ‘cold’ cognitive processes, possibly including Bayesian updating, to maintain or exacerbate motivated differences in beliefs, opinions and values61,102,103,104,105. As a result, they may exhibit cognitive rigidity — psychological inflexibility and a failure to adapt to novel environments and unfamiliar ways of thinking106,107.

Finally, when it comes to democratic tolerance and other key outcomes, there are meaningful ideological asymmetries that should not be ignored. Whereas liberal-leftists are more willing to challenge the societal status quo and push for egalitarian forms of social change, conservative-rightists are more authoritarian and more protective of cultural traditions as well as longstanding social, economic and political institutions101,107,149,166,168,172. These differences in beliefs, opinions and values might well create asymmetries in all three forms of polarization, but more research is needed to link individual and group differences in system justification to political outcomes such as communication, persuasion, polarization and democratic commitment. Studies along these lines would help to illuminate the causes and consequences of dramatic asymmetries in the attitudes and behaviours of liberal-leftists and conservative-rightists in the USA and elsewhere10,34,47,48,73,74,75,161,167,185,186,187,310,311,312,313,314,315.

Stanley, M. L., Henne, P., Yang, B. W. & De Brigard, F. Resistance to position change, motivated reasoning, and polarization. Polit. Behav. 42, 891–913 (2020).

Effron, D. A. It could have been true: how counterfactual thoughts reduce condemnation of falsehoods and increase political polarization. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 44, 729–745 (2018).

Polarization examples

Bishop, B. The Big Sort: Why the Clustering of Like-Minded America Is Tearing Us Apart (Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2009).

Sieber, J. & Ziegler, R. Group polarization revisited: a processing effort account. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 45, 1482–1498 (2019).

Partisan alignment can also influence affective polarization. For example, citizens who take more issue positions on the same side of the ideological spectrum as their party (that is, are more aligned) exhibit more out-group animus and affective polarization, adjusting for many other factors84.

Broockman, D. E., Kalla, J. L. & Westwood S. J. Does affective polarization undermine democratic norms or accountability? Am. J. Polit. Sci. (in the press).

Binocular goggles: Binocular goggles are the more common way of working. It has two lenses called the field mirror and the eyepiece. This design allows the observer to observe the specimen using both eyes, simulating the natural observation environment and reducing eye strain.

Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A. & Bonneau, R. Tweeting from left to right: is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychol. Sci. 26, 1531–1542 (2015). This study estimates ideological preferences of 3.8 million Twitter users in the USA and finds that ideological segregation in social media is less extreme than previously thought. Moreover, liberals are more likely than conservatives to engage in cross-ideological dissemination of information online.

Van Boven, L., Judd, C. M. & Sherman, D. K. Political polarization projection: social projection of partisan attitude extremity and attitudinal processes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 84 (2012).

Merkley, E. & Stecula, D. A. Party cues in the news: democratic elites, Republican backlash, and the dynamics of climate skepticism. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 51, 1439–1456 (2021).

Levendusky, M. & Malhotra, N. Does media coverage of partisan polarization affect political attitudes? Polit. Commun. 33, 283–301 (2016).

Osborne, D., Jost, J. T., Becker, J., Badaan, V. & Sibley, C. G. Protesting to challenge or defend the system? A system justification perspective on collective action. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 49, 244–269 (2019).

Eibach, R. Ideological polarization and social psychology. Oxford Res. Encyc. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.240 (2021).

Bail, C. A. et al. Exposure to opposing views on social media can increase political polarization. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 9216–9221 (2018).

Pierson, P. & Schickler, E. Madison’s constitution under stress: a developmental analysis of political polarization. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 23, 37–58 (2020).

Lee, B. & Bearman, P. Political isolation in America. Netw. Sci. 8, 333–355 (2020). The study documents trends in the composition of political discussion networks and shows that American’s core discussion networks shrink during heated political times.

Van Bavel, J. J. & Packer, D. The Power of Us: Harnessing Our Shared Identities to Improve Performance, Increase Cooperation, and Promote Social Harmony (Little Brown Spark, 2021).

Grossmann, M. & Hopkins, D. A. Asymmetric Politics: Ideological Republicans and Group Interest Democrats (Oxford Univ. Press, 2016).

First, issue or ideological polarization (Fig. 1a) is characterized as moving towards the extremes and away from the centre with respect to issues and/or ideology29. This type of polarization is typically operationalized as approaching or arriving at a bimodal distribution of beliefs, opinions and values50,72. This implies either that two groups — such as liberals and conservatives or leftists and rightists — are moving in opposite, more extreme directions (symmetric polarization) or that one group is becoming more extreme than the other (asymmetric polarization10,34,47,48,73,74,75).

Ravndal, J. A. Explaining right-wing terrorism and violence in Western Europe: grievances, opportunities and polarisation. Eur. J. Polit. Res. 57, 845–866 (2018).

van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A. & Maibach, E. The gateway belief model: a large-scale replication. J. Environ. Psychol. 62, 49–58 (2019).

Layman, G. C., Carsey, T. M., Green, J. C., Herrera, R. & Cooperman, R. Activists and conflict extension in American party politics. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 104, 324–346 (2010).

Lipset, S. M. & Rokkan, S. in Party Systems and Voter Alignments: Cross-National Perspectives 1–64 (The Free Press, 1967).

Crowson, H. M. & Brandes, J. A. Differentiating between Donald Trump and Hillary Clinton voters using facets of right-wing authoritarianism and social-dominance orientation: a brief report. Psychol. Rep. 120, 364–373 (2017).

Many people avoid interpersonal conflict, which can be ego-damaging, without permanently modifying their social network. For example, during close election campaigns, people eschew political conversations with adversaries by skipping family reunions or shortening Thanksgiving dinners231. When contact is unavoidable, people engage in selective disclosure232,233. That is, they simply withhold their attitudes from people with whom they expect to disagree. This could result in pluralistic ignorance (an exaggerated appearance of homogeneity) because only certain political views are expressed, and disagreement is suppressed233,234,235,236. Thus, even among people who maintain reasonably heterogeneous online and offline relationships, ego-defensive mechanisms such as selective disclosure145 (and selective censorship) could undermine the depolarizing potential of these networks. These mechanisms might also reinforce in-group norms by concealing within-group variability, potentially exacerbating other forms of alignment and polarization143,237.

Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E. & Slothuus, R. How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 107, 57–79 (2013).

People who hold more liberal attitudes may have more ideologically diverse online networks than people who hold more conservative attitudes207,228. Furthermore, an analysis of over 20,000 responses to the 2016 National Consumer Survey revealed that self-identified liberals in the USA are more culturally omnivorous than self-identified conservatives, insofar as they report a larger number and variety of cultural exposures in a wide range of domains, including movies, TV shows, live performances, music, magazines, websites, hobbies and beer brands, even after adjusting for the effects of age, income and regional differences in population density217,229. These results imply that, because of greater openness to new experiences217,230 and more diverse social networks207,228, individuals who embrace liberal ideology are more familiar with and knowledgeable about the cultural activities of out-group members than are individuals who embrace conservative ideology.

Abramowitz, A. I. Transformation and polarization: the 2008 presidential election and the new American electorate. Elect. Stud. 29, 594–603 (2010).

However, there is clearly a market for political misinformation255, and technology companies such as Twitter and Facebook profit from the spread of ‘fake news’, which increases customer engagement and therefore advertising revenue256. These market incentives both reflect and exacerbate ideological and affective forms of polarization, insofar as emotionally charged material, such as that which is contemptuous of ideological adversaries, is more likely to go ‘viral’ than more measured types of political discourse260,263,269,270.

Polarization politics

Van Bavel, J. J. & Pereira, A. The partisan brain: an identity-based model of political belief. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 213–224 (2018).

Rathje, S., Van Bavel, J. J. & van der Linden, S. Out-group animosity drives engagement on social media. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2024292118 (2021). This study of social media platforms finds that messages expressing negative emotions (such as anger, moral outrage and mockery) about the ideological out-group were especially likely to be shared on Facebook and Twitter, compared to other types of message.

Duckitt, J. A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33, 41–113 (2001).

Boxell, L., Gentzkow, M. & Shapiro, J. M. Greater internet use is not associated with faster growth in political polarization among US demographic groups. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 10612–10617 (2017).

Halliez, A. A. & Thornton, J. R. Examining trends in ideological identification: 1972–2016. Am. Polit. Res. 49, 259–268 (2021).

Wolf, L. J., Weinstein, N. & Maio, G. R. Anti-immigrant prejudice: understanding the roles of (perceived) values and value dissimilarity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 117, 925–953 (2019).

Modern eyepieces usually employ compound lenses to correct optical defects such as magnification chromatic aberration, coma, and aberration. The design of the field mirror and eyepiece allows the observer to obtain high quality microscopic images.

Message frames can shift the salience of group identities and therefore the basis for group-justification. For example, Democratic parents express very different social and political attitudes when their status as parents (as compared to their status as Democrats) is made salient280. The parental frame resulted in issue depolarization: Democrats expressed attitudes that were more similar to those of Republicans when their parental identification was made salient compared to various other experimental conditions (such as when their partisan identity was made salient). Likewise, emphasizing a superordinate national identity can reduce out-group animus as well as affective and ideological polarization281,282,283, consistent with social identity theory90,92,139.

The body of literature on mass political polarization in the USA has yielded mixed findings: ordinary citizens are deeply divided9,31,35,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49 or they are not10,29,36,50,51,52,53,54; polarization poses a huge threat to democratic functioning3,11,55, is not a problem at all13,56,57,58 or the nature of the threat is ambiguous7,59,60. These mixed findings reflect a literature that has been vague in identifying precise psychological mechanisms and how they operate across social-communicative contexts61.

Glinitzer, K., Gummer & Wagner, M. Learning facts about migration: politically motivated learning of polarizing information about refugees. Polit. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12734 (2021).

Furthermore, our conceptual framework suggests a blueprint for how to proceed. First, scholars must identify which circumstances are likely to trigger ego-justifying, group-justifying and system-justifying motives99,101,299. Second, although source, message, channel, receiver and target variables have been studied for approximately 70 years188,189, we still know fairly little about how they interact with one another. For example, researchers should seek to understand how people reconcile conflicting information coming from political elites, social media platforms and face-to-face interaction with friends, and the degree to which the impact of specific narratives or message frames depends upon the source, the channel and the receiver.

Klofstad, C. A., McDermott, R. & Hatemi, P. K. The dating preferences of liberals and conservatives. Polit. Behav. 35, 519–538 (2013).

Azevedo, F., Jost, J. T. & Rothmund, T. “Making America great again”: system justification in the 2016 U.S. presidential election. Transl. Issues Psychol. Sci. 3, 231–240 (2017).

Webster, S. W. & Abramowitz, A. I. The ideological foundations of affective polarization in the US electorate. Am. Polit. Res. 45, 621–647 (2017). This article uses survey and experimental data to show that ideological beliefs (polarization) influence affective polarization, especially when it comes to social welfare issues.

In this Review, we provide a conceptual framework for organizing the literature on mass political polarization that can help scholars and practitioners to understand the conditional nature of different types of polarization better, and whether they stem from normatively concerning dynamics. We first describe the three major operational definitions of mass polarization and their relations to each other. Then, we describe the cognitive–motivational mechanisms that give rise to political polarization, and how these play out in social-communicative contexts to increase or decrease the degree of political polarization in society.

The ideological homogeneity of social networks is enhanced by structural dynamics such as geographic sorting209,210,211, the growing politicization of the rural–urban divide212,213, and ‘lifestyle’ differences that are correlated with political orientation107,214,215,216 (see also Box 1). These geographic and cultural divisions are likely to limit exposure to ‘the other side’, especially among individuals who hold more conservative attitudes and are less open to new or different experiences217.

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Jeff Lees, Konstantin Vossing and Leor Zmigrod for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Flores, A. et al. Politicians polarize and experts depolarize public support for COVID-19 management policies across countries. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2117543119 (2022).

Drummond, C. & Fischhoff, B. Does “putting on your thinking cap” reduce myside bias in evaluation of scientific evidence? Think. Reason. 25, 477–505 (2019).

Mass political polarization occurs when individuals become more divided in their political views or develop stronger, more intense group attachments. There are three operational definitions of mass political polarization (see Fig. 1), which might differ in terms of whether they are psychologically underpinned by cognitive, motivational or affective concerns.

Brady, W. J., Wills, J., Jost, J. T., Tucker, J. & Van Bavel, J. J. Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 7313–7318 (2017). This study shows that moral-emotional language in political messages diffuse at high rates within (but not between) ideological groups on social media, showing how social media can affect polarization through immersion in an ideological network.

Vössing, K. Shaping public opinion about regional integration: the rhetoric of justification and party cues. Polit. Stud. 69, 492–513 (2021).

Following political elites and being ‘good group members’ enables citizens to satisfy ego-justifying, group-justifying and (in some cases) system-justifying motivations. When elites signal ideological disagreement and/or out-group animus, followers get the message and behave accordingly. Consistent with this theoretical logic, several experiments have demonstrated that representing political elites as highly polarized (either in terms of ideological abstractions or specific issue positions) leads participants to express higher levels of issue-based and affective polarization39,83. Importantly, when the same political elites are described as less divergent, participants exhibit more overlap in their own policy opinions141. Exposing people to warm interactions between political elites from opposing parties also seems to reduce out-group animosity58,199.

Mercier, H. in Cognitive Illusions: Intriguing Phenomena In Thinking, Judgment And Memory (ed. Pohl, R. F.) 99–114 (Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group, 2017).

Franco, A. B. & Pound, N. The foundations of Bolsonaro’s support: exploring the psychological underpinnings of political polarization in Brazil. J. Commun. Appl. Soc. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2599 (2022).

Hing, L. S. S., Wilson, A. E., Gourevitch, P., English, J. & Sin, P. Failure to respond to rising income inequality: processes that legitimize growing disparities. Daedalus 148, 105–135 (2019).

Vitriol, J. & Moskowitz, G. B. Reducing defensive responding to implicit bias feedback: on the role of perceived moral threat and efficacy to change. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 96, 104165 (2021).

Martin, G. J. & Webster, S. W. Does residential sorting explain geographic polarization? Polit. Sci. Res. Meth. 8, 215–231 (2020).

Cross-national comparisons of political polarization are complicated by variations in political systems and cleavage alignments. In the USA, there are three major cleavages on economic, civil rights and moral issues, with the moral dimension largely crosscutting the other two dimensions40,77. In European countries, there are longstanding territorial, religious and social class cleavages and more recent cleavages pertaining to globalization and integration with the European Union333,334. Despite these differences, the cognitive–motivational and social-communicative processes that contribute to polarization seem to be largely the same in the USA and Europe.

Hartman, R. et al. Interventions to reduce political animosity: a systematic review. Trends Cogn. Sci. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ha2tf (2021).

We focus on source, message and channel characteristics, because their relevance to group polarization is especially evident in the Internet era. There are also important receiver factors107 (such as the ideological composition of the audience and their psychological characteristics) and target factors (such as whether the goal of the persuasive communication is to convince people to vote for a specific candidate or to storm the US Capitol building). Furthermore, in Box 1 we discuss how behavioural norms, which need not involve explicit forms of communication, can affect different types of political polarization.

Although it is beyond the purview of our analysis, it is important to keep in mind that levels of polarization (and their political consequences) depend on a number of contextual factors65. For instance, polarization led parties in power to enact constitutional reforms to consolidate their power in Hungary, Turkey and Venezuela, whereas power grabs in the USA occurred through the violation of democratic norms and refusal to compromise11.

Jost, J. T., van der Linden, S., Panagopoulos, C. & Hardin, C. D. Ideological asymmetries in conformity, desire for shared reality, and the spread of misinformation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 23, 77–83 (2018). From the perspective of system justification theory, this article reviews evidence of ideological asymmetry such that conservatives prioritize conformity, possess a stronger desire for a shared reality with those who share their ideology, and maintain more homogeneous networks, compared to liberals.

Hopkins, D. J. & Ladd, J. M. The consequences of broader media choice: evidence from the expansion of Fox News. Q. J. Polit. Sci. 9, 115–135 (2014).

Importantly, individuals play an active part in selecting and navigating their social networks. Much network homophily — the tendency for people to associate with similar others — is based on socio-demographic characteristics (such as race, education, income, age and gender). At the same time, there is evidence that ideological and partisan considerations inform the choice of friends, as well as business and romantic partners218,219,220,221. It is not entirely clear whether people are explicitly choosing interaction partners on the basis of political considerations or whether ideological homophily is a byproduct of associational patterns rooted in socio-demographic similarities59. This is an important question because the implications for polarization differ.

Second, mass polarization in terms of partisan alignment (Fig. 1b) focuses on the dimensionality of the political space. Political parties are usually based on social and/or cultural cleavages62,64,76. In pluralistic political systems, there are often crosscutting cleavages, such as those based on social class, region or religion. These cleavages might provide opportunities for cooperation on some issues, such as when poor members of different religious groups work together on economic issues, while dividing alliances on other issues, such as moral questions, for which cleavages based on religion may trump those based on class. Sometimes multiple lines of potential disagreement in society become highly overlapping, aligned or consolidated. In these cases, multiple potential cleavages might be reduced to a single dimension that comes to dominate political relations8,72,77,78. Put another way, members of a group (such as a political party) might come to share similar views across many different issues, so that these issue positions do not crosscut group membership. Operationally, researchers often measure partisan alignment in terms of consistency (or ‘constraint’) among issue domains (for example, whether correlations among attitudes on economic, civil rights and moral issues have increased or decreased). Thus, extreme differences between groups might arise not from movement on a single dimension (such as left–right ideology) but from the alignment or consolidation of multiple dimensions77, such that the same constituencies (or segments of society) are in repeated disagreement over many issues. Intense conflict might arise when many possible differences of opinion reduce to a single ‘us versus them’ dimension11.

Raymond, L., Kelly, D. & Hennes, E. Norm-based governance for a new era: collective action in the face of hyper-politicization. Persp. Polit. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592721003054 (2021).

Allcott, H. et al. Polarization and public health: partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. J. Public Econ. 191, 104254 (2020).

Polarization in Chemistry

Mudde, C. Fighting the system? Populist radical right parties and party system change. Party Politics 20, 217–226 (2014).

Lupton, R. N., Smallpage, S. M. & Enders, A. M. Values and political predispositions in the age of polarization: examining the relationship between partisanship and ideology in the United States, 1988–2012. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 50, 1–20 (2017). This study uses over-time data to show that alignment between ideology and partisanship has increased asymmetrically among those with conservative value orientations, presumably owing to Republican discourse emphasizing traditional family values and resistance to social change.

Likewise, ideological polarization in general — not just on specific issues or values — can drive affective polarization. In a large-scale experiment, participants read about two hypothetical political candidates who were represented as either slightly liberal and slightly conservative (in the convergent condition) or as very liberal and very conservative (divergent condition). In feeling thermometer ratings, participants were more affectively polarized in the divergent condition, expressing more warmth towards the more extreme candidate whose ideology they shared and less warmth towards the more extreme candidate whose ideology they did not share. This effect was stronger for participants who were more interested in politics and more ideologically extreme themselves83. Another series of experiments demonstrated that learning about policy disagreements between rank-and-file Democrats and Republicans made partisan identities more salient and caused participants to express more warmth towards in-party members and less warmth towards out-party members81.

Xu, X., Mar, R. A. & Peterson, J. B. Does cultural exposure partially explain the association between personality and political orientation? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39, 1497–1517 (2013).

Baldassarri, D. & Gelman, A. Partisans without constraint: political polarization and trends in American public opinion. Am. J. Sociol. 114, 408–446 (2008).

The concept of polarization is also used to characterize states or trends in the conditions of intergroup relations. That is, polarization may refer to a large or increasing gap between two or more groups. This has clear relevance to politics, which often involves group-level competition over policy issues and/or ideologies. For example, there is a strong consensus among scholars of politics in the USA that liberal and conservative political elites (including members of Congress, party activists, donors and judicial appointees) have grown increasingly polarized over the past few decades27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34. At the same time, the extent to which elite-level polarization has influenced the belief systems of ordinary citizens remains a matter of debate8,27,35,36,37,38,39,40.

Craig, M. A., Rucker, J. M. & Richeson, J. A. The pitfalls and promise of increasing racial diversity: threat, contact, and race relations in the 21st century. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 188–193 (2018). This review provides a framework for studying the impact of the growth of racial and ethnic diversity, and discusses how demographic changes can generate status threat and prejudice but could also lead to positive interpersonal contact experiences.

Brown, J. R., Enos, R. D., Feigenbaum, J. & Mazumder, S. Childhood cross-ethnic exposure predicts political behavior seven decades later: evidence from linked administrative data. Sci. Adv. 7, eabe8432 (2021).

Boxell, L., Conway, J., Druckman, J. N. & Gentzkow, M. Affective polarization did not increase during the COVID-19 pandemic. Q. J. Polit. Sci. Forthcoming https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3785328 (2021).

Just as issue or ideological polarization and partisan alignment can exacerbate affective polarization, affective polarization can exacerbate issue or ideological polarization and partisan alignment. For instance, people were more favourable towards a policy proposal when it was described as coming from the in-party rather than the out-party85, suggesting that general group-based attitudes drove issue preferences, thereby leading to issue polarization. However, follow-up research indicated that this was true for Republican but not Democratic supporters86.

Baldassarri, D. & Goldberg, A. Neither ideologues nor agnostics: alternative voters’ belief system in an age of partisan politics. Am. J. Sociol. 120, 45–95 (2014). This network analysis of political belief systems of USA voters finds that a third of the electorate is composed of individuals who are morally conservative but economically liberal, or vice versa. These conflicting political views are linked to sociodemographic profiles.

Goya-Tocchetto, D., Kay, A. C., Vuletich, H., Vonasch, A. & Payne, K. The partisan trade-off bias: when political polarization meets policy trade-offs. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 98, 104231 (2022).

Layman, G. C. & Carsey, T. M. Party polarization and “conflict extension” in the American electorate. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 46, 786–802 (2002).

Cialdini, R. B., Levy, A., Herman, C. P. & Evenbeck, S. Attitudinal politics: the strategy of moderation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 25, 100–108 (1973).

Representations of nationalism and national identification might serve as ‘master frames’ that shape collective self-understanding285,286. Prior to 2000, Democrats and Republicans differed little in terms of nationalist beliefs, but as of 2016 they held very different conceptions, apparently because of 9/11, the wars in Afghanistan and Iraq, and racial resentment in response to Barack Obama’s presidency286. By framing patriotism in politically conservative terms, Republican elites appear to have increased the alignment between Republican partisanship and nationalist values. Experimental studies suggest that increasing system justification motivation may, in some cases, raise the national attachment levels of people who hold liberal views so that they more closely resemble those of people who hold conservative views, thereby temporarily reducing polarization on issues of patriotism — but not nationalism, defined as an attitude of superiority towards other countries282,287,288,289.

Ahler, D. J. & Sood, G. The parties in our heads: misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. J. Polit. 80, 964–981 (2018).

In summary, the cylinder and eyepiece play a crucial role in microscopy, and their precise design and working mode provide a better viewing experience and high-quality microscopic observation results. These key components allow scientists to delve deeper into the microscopic world and reveal its mysteries. The mirror tube and the eyepiece together form the window of the microscopic world.

Tajfel, H. & Turner, J. C. in Psychology of Intergroup Relations (eds Worchel, S. & Austin, W. A.) 7–24 (Nelson-Hall, 1986).

The concept of group polarization comes from experimental social psychology. It refers to the oft-observed tendency for members of a social group to arrive at a consensus (through group discussion and other forms of social interaction) that is more extreme but in the same direction as the average of their initial opinions on a given issue19. For example, groups that start out mildly risk-seeking become more risk-seeking over time, whereas groups that start out mildly cautious become more cautious over time. There are two classes of explanation for this phenomenon. The first has to do with persuasive argumentation: during group discussion, people are exposed to additional arguments in favour of the position that the average group member was already inclined to take19,20. The second focuses on social comparison processes and conformity to perceived group norms: individuals in a group conform to what they perceive as the group norm. This perception, however, might be exaggerated to maintain group distinctiveness and enhance contrast with other groups21,22,23,24. Thus, from a social psychological perspective, group polarization is a dynamic property of a single social group — a shifting of group behaviour over time in a more extreme direction, but in the same direction in which the group started. It is not, strictly speaking, a property of individuals, although the concept of polarization has been extended, by analogy, to individual-level processes, such as the tendency for individuals to become more extreme in their own thinking25 or the strength of their group identification26.

Talaifar, S. & Swann, W. B., Jr. Deep alignment with country shrinks the moral gap between conservatives and liberals. Polit. Psychol. 40, 657–675 (2019).

Baldassarri, D. & Park, B. Was there a culture war? Partisan polarization and secular trends in US public opinion. J. Polit. 82, 809–827 (2020). This analysis of trends in public opinion in the USA over time finds partisan polarization on economic and civil rights issues, whereas opinions on moral issues followed a trend of secularization. Both Democrats and Republicans have increasingly adopted more progressive moral views, but Republicans changed their views more slowly than Democrats.

Brady, W. J., Wills, J. A., Burkart, D., Jost, J. T. & Van Bavel, J. J. An ideological asymmetry in the diffusion of moralized content on social media among political leaders. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148, 1802–1813 (2019).

Barberá, P. in Social Media and Democracy: The State of the Field (eds Persily, N. & Tucker, J.) (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2020).

Cohen, G. L. Party over policy: the dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 808–822 (2003).

Jost, J. T., Becker, J., Osborne, D. & Badaan, V. Missing in (collective) action: ideology, system justification, and the motivational antecedents of protest behavior. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 99–108 (2017).

Finally, valence framing (highlighting positive or negative aspects or themes) can contribute to polarization or depolarization under certain circumstances. For example, when multiple media sources are available, negative campaigning against one’s political opponent — compared to positive campaigning for oneself — increases affective polarization290. At the same time, exposure to overly confrontational, uncivil discourse from members of one’s own party can lead to depolarization; this is because citizens view such rhetoric as norm-violating and wish to distance themselves from the source of the message249.

Krumrei-Mancuso, E. J. & Newman, B. Intellectual humility in the sociopolitical domain. Self Identity 19, 989–1016 (2020).

Vraga, E. K. How party affiliation conditions the experience of dissonance and explains polarization and selective exposure. Soc. Sci. Q. 96, 487–502 (2015).

Eastwick, P. W., Richeson, J. A., Son, D. & Finkel, E. J. Is love colorblind? Political orientation and interracial romantic desire. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 1258–1268 (2009).

Torcal, M. & Magalhães, P. C. Ideological extremism, perceived party system polarization, and support for democracy. Eur. Polit. Sci. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755773922000066 (2022).

Perez-Truglia, R. & Cruces, G. Partisan interactions: evidence from a field experiment in the United States. J. Polit. Econ. 125, 1208–1243 (2017).

Polarization of light

Iyengar, S., Sood, G. & Lelkes, Y. Affect, not ideology: a social identity perspective on polarization. Public. Opin. Q. 76, 405–431 (2012).

Hacker, J. & Pierson, P. in Solutions to Political Polarization in America (ed. Persily, N.) 59–70 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2015).

Political communication often takes a narrative format in which events are described in chronological order alongside information about key characters and their actions190,291. Many of these narratives provide moral lessons or takeaways292. For instance, narratives about marginalized groups, such as struggle-oriented stories about immigrants, can counteract system justification tendencies that might otherwise discount or disparage immigrants’ experiences293,294,295. In addition, giving people an opportunity to participate in a ‘non-judgemental exchange of narratives’ was found to increase feelings of warmth towards unauthorized immigrants and transgender people, compared to various control conditions (no opportunity for exchange and/or participation in a brief, unrelated conversation). The narrative exchange also increased support for inclusive public policies, compared to control conditions, and therefore reduced issue polarization296.

Kriesi, H. et al. Globalization and the transformation of the national political space: six European countries compared. Eur. J. Polit. Res. 45, 921–956 (2006).

Enders, A. M. & Lupton, R. N. Value extremity contributes to affective polarization in the US. Polit. Sci. Res. Meth. 9, 857–866 (2021).

Howell, J. L., Gaither, S. E. & Ratliff, K. A. Caught in the middle: defensive responses to IAT feedback among whites, blacks, and biracial black/whites. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 6, 373–381 (2015).

Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J. & Steele, C. M. When beliefs yield to evidence: reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 1151–1164 (2000).

Because citizens take social cues from political elites and are sometimes highly influenced by them191,192, it is important to understand whether and how elite-level polarization spreads to ordinary citizens8,35,36,50,193. The preponderance of evidence suggests that it does. Elite polarization predated the rise of mass polarization in the USA by several years, suggesting that voters began to ‘catch up’ to party leaders over time10,42,48. This is true with respect to ideology in general as well as with respect to controversial issues such as climate change194,195, abortion196 and COVID-19 policies197. To be clear, we are not suggesting that citizens are merely passive sponges, mindlessly absorbing elite messages. Rather, consistent with cognitive response theories of persuasion188,198, receivers actively respond to political communication, in accordance with their own psychological needs, motives and abilities107.

van der Linden, S., Panagopoulos, C., Azevedo, F. & Jost, J. T. The paranoid style in American politics revisited: an ideological asymmetry in conspiratorial thinking. Polit. Psychol. 42, 23–51 (2021).

Nosek, B., Banaji, M. R., & Jost, J. T. in Social and Psychological Bases of Ideology and System Justification (eds Jost, J. T., Kay, A. C. & Thorisdottir, H.) 480–506 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

Morisi, D., Jost, J. T. & Singh, V. An asymmetrical “president-in-power” effect. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 113, 614–620 (2019). This research shows that US conservatives’ trust in government, more than liberals’, is shaped substantially by who holds the presidency — they trust the government much more when the president shares their ideology.

Ashokkumar, A., Galaif, M. & Swann, W. B. Jr Tribalism can corrupt: why people denounce or protect immoral group members. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 85, 103874 (2019).

Some studies suggest that social media platforms are frequently used to spread out-group animosity, thereby exacerbating affective polarization261. For example, an analysis of nearly 600,000 Facebook posts and more than 200,000 Twitter posts by media outlets on the political left (such as the New York Times and MSNBC) and political right (such as Fox News and Breitbart) found that posts mentioning the political in-group were shared more often than other posts (with estimated increases in diffusion rate ranging from 0% to 37%). However, posts mentioning the out-group were even more likely to be shared (with estimated diffusion rate increases ranging from 29% to 57%260). Sentiment analysis of messages about the out-group revealed that they frequently expressed negative emotions such as anger, moral outrage and mockery. A follow-up study of more than 800 Facebook posts and a million Twitter posts from the official accounts of members of the US Congress showed that messages mentioning the out-group, but not the in-group, were more likely to be shared than other posts (increased diffusion rates ranging from 58% to 180%260). Retweets often included anger-related language260; this is important because anger inflames political polarization by, among other things, increasing cognitive oversimplification and categorical thinking that divides ‘us’ and ‘them.’132.

Huddy, L. & Yair, O. Reducing affective polarization: warm group relations or policy compromise? Polit. Psychol. 42, 291–309 (2021).

Social media platforms not only amplify the impact of messages transmitted by elite sources through mass media. They also create new and unprecedented opportunities for friends, acquaintances, and other members of extended social networks to share information and misinformation — as well as emotional and motivational appeals, including those grounded in shared group identities and ideologies — in overtly political contexts254.

Prior, M. Post-Broadcast Democracy: How Media Choice Increases Inequality In Political Involvement And Polarizes Elections (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2007).

Druckman, J. N. & Nelson, K. R. Framing and deliberation: how citizens’ conversations limit elite influence. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 47, 729–745 (2003).

Dunning, D. A newer look: motivated social cognition and the schematic representation of social concepts. Psychol. Inq. 10, 1–11 (1999).

Some degree of ideological debate and partisan separation is to be expected and might be desirable to ensure that citizens’ voices are well-represented by political elites3,5,6,7. At the same time, intense, seemingly intractable political conflict along multiple lines of divergence might threaten national unity and constructive opportunities for compromise8,9,10,11,12,13. One clearly deleterious consequence of polarization is that urgent matters that need not become politicized — such as addressing the public health crisis instigated by COVID-19 and individual decisions about mask-wearing and vaccination — become extremely contentious, leading to serious failures of social cooperation14,15,16,17,18.

Porat, R., Tamir, M., Wohl, M. J., Gur, T. & Halperin, E. Motivated emotion and the rally around the flag effect: liberals are motivated to feel collective angst (like conservatives) when faced with existential threat. Cogn. Emot. 33, 480–491 (2019).

Bakshy, E., Messing, S. & Adamic, L. A. Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook. Science 348, 1130–1132 (2015).

McCoy, J., Rahman, T. & Somer, M. Polarization and the global crisis of democracy: common patterns, dynamics, and pernicious consequences for democratic politics. Am. Behav. Sci. 62, 16–42 (2018).

Linear polarization

Elite signals are typically communicated to citizens through mass media channels that have changed immensely since the advent of cable television and the ideological segmentation of news content238. Politically engaged viewers, who are more attentive to elite cues191, have increasingly tuned into cable and Internet news sources239,240,241,242, while others have dropped out243,244. Exposure to ideologically driven networks (such as Fox News on the political right and MSNBC on the political left) exerts both persuasive245,246 and reinforcement effects247. The former occurs when, for instance, Independents who view Fox News are convinced to support a Republican candidate. The latter occurs when Republicans who view Fox News become even more likely to vote for a Republican candidate. Partisan media exposure also increases issue polarization, especially among viewers who are already fairly extreme in their views248.

In terms of research interventions, reducing ego-defensiveness by encouraging people to affirm positive things about themselves sometimes helps to reduce information-processing biases129,130. Other intervention strategies focus on correcting misperceptions, encouraging more complex thinking, and reducing anger and competitive feelings26,131,132. Some of these techniques (reducing anger and competition) might reduce ego-defensive motivation itself, whereas others (correcting misperceptions) seek to undo the consequences of ego-defensive processing. However, the latter run the risk of provoking additional efforts at ego-justification if they are not implemented carefully and subtly133,134. Finally, cultivating a spirit of intellectual humility (responding generously and humbly towards others) could help to reduce political polarization by reducing ego-defensiveness135,136,137,138. However, these studies focused on relatively short-term changes; more research is needed to develop interventions that promote lasting forms of change.

Azevedo, F., Jost, J. T., Rothmund, T. & Sterling, J. Neoliberal ideology and the justification of inequality in capitalist societies: why social and economic dimensions of ideology are intertwined. J. Soc. Issues 75, 49–88 (2019).

Bonikowski, B. Ethno-nationalist populism and the mobilization of collective resentment. Br. J. Sociol. 68, S181–S213 (2017).

Prior, M., Sood, G. & Khanna, K. You cannot be serious: the impact of accuracy incentives on partisan bias in reports of economic perceptions. Q. J. Polit. Sci. 10, 489–518 (2015).

Kozlowski, A. C. & Murphy, J. P. Issue alignment and partisanship in the American public: revisiting the ‘partisans without constraint’ thesis. Soc. Sci. Res. 94, 1024–98 (2021).

Mummolo, J. & Nall, C. Why partisans do not sort: the constraints on political segregation. J. Polit. 79, 45–59 (2017). Evidence from survey experiments suggests that partisans are not migrating to more politically distinct communities. By prioritizing common concerns when deciding where to live, Americans forgo the opportunity to move to more politically congenial communities.

McGarty, C., Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., David, B. & Wetherell, M. S. Group polarization as conformity to the prototypical group member. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 31, 1–19 (1992).

Thus, all three types of polarization (issue or ideological polarization, partisan alignment and affective polarization) can lead citizens to understand politics and society in terms of ‘us versus them’, with potentially deleterious consequences for intergroup relations. A major conclusion of our Review is that these different types of polarization can become mutually reinforcing. For example, analysis of data from the American National Election Studies from 1984 to 2012 showed that attitudes about social welfare policy and, to a lesser extent, abortion and gay rights, were strongly predictive of negative feelings about the opposing party and its presidential candidates48. This suggests that issue polarization can fuel affective polarization. In another research programme, polarization on values in the first wave of a survey predicted affective polarization four years later, adjusting for value polarization during the second wave82.

van der Toorn, J., Nail, P., Liviatan, I. & Jost, J. T. My country, right or wrong: does activating system justification motivation eliminate the liberal–conservative gap in patriotism? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 54, 50–60 (2014).

Vegetti, F. The political nature of ideological polarization: the case of Hungary. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 681, 78–96 (2019).

The cylinder is the bottom part of the microscope, usually connected to the objective lens, and can be divided into three types: monocular, binocular, and trinary.

Robison, J. & Mullinix, K. J. Elite polarization and public opinion: how polarization is communicated and its effects. Polit. Commun. 33, 261–282 (2016).

Dubois, E. & Blank, G. The echo chamber is overstated: the moderating effect of political interest and diverse media. Inf. Commun. Soc. 21, 729–745 (2018).

Druckman, J. N. & McGrath, M. C. The evidence for motivated reasoning in climate change preference formation. Nat. Clim. Change 9, 111–119 (2019). This work highlights the difficulty of distinguishing partisan motivated reasoning from accuracy-driven reasoning, noting that most studies assume but do not show that motivated reasoning exacerbates issue polarization.

Hutchens, M. J., Hmielowski, J. D. & Beam, M. A. Reinforcing spirals of political discussion and affective polarization. Commun. Monogr. 86, 357–376 (2019).

Moore-Berg, S. L., Hameiri, B. & Bruneau, E. The prime psychological suspects of toxic political polarization. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 199–204 (2020).

Whereas ego-justifying and group-justifying motives contribute to relatively symmetrical forms of polarization on the liberal-left and conservative-right, a third class of cognitive–motivational mechanisms related to system justification might contribute to ideological asymmetries99,149. This is because system justification motivation — defined as the (not necessarily conscious) system-serving tendency to defend, bolster and legitimize aspects of the societal status quo — is generally correlated with political conservatism101. Consequently, those who endorse conservative attitudes exhibit stronger biases in favour of the status quo than those who endorse liberal attitudes. This asymmetry can have consequences for affective polarization, including stronger preferences for incumbent candidates and chief executives from one’s own party (the ‘President-in-power’ effect) on the part of conservative (versus liberal) voters161,162. In addition, decades of research in political psychology demonstrate that those who embrace more conservative-rightist attitudes, identities and leaders are more authoritarian and social-dominance-oriented (that is, supportive of group-based hierarchies) than those who embrace more liberal-leftist attitudes, identities and leaders107,149,163,164,165,166,167,168,169. It is likely that respondents who believe that “What our country really needs is a strong, determined President who will crush the evil and set us in our right way again” (an item from the right-wing authoritarianism scale) and “Some groups of people are simply inferior to other groups” (an item from the social-dominance orientation scale), who identify as more conservative or rightist in political orientation, are more susceptible to out-group animus and affective polarization than the more liberal and leftist respondents who reject these sentiments166,170.

Garcia-Rada, X. & Norton, M. I. Putting within-country political differences in (global) perspective. PLoS ONE 15, e0231794 (2021).

Finally, affective polarization (Fig. 1c) occurs when members of different social groups (or political parties) hold strong positive or negative attitudes (or feelings) about the groups themselves (not just their policy preferences49,79), owing to ‘in-party love’ and/or ‘out-party animus’80. (It may be that people do not necessarily hold negative attitudes towards out-party members in particular, but rather towards any person or group that is highly partisan54.) Operationally, affective polarization is often measured with the use of ‘feeling thermometers’ that tap warmth versus coldness towards political parties or ideological groups. Here the focus is not necessarily on issues (such as taxes or abortion) or ideology (liberal versus conservative) but rather on affective group evaluations. In principle, one could hold extremely conservative views without hating the Democratic party or vice versa81.

On other issues, the direction of asymmetric polarization is reversed. For instance, support for governmental regulation of the economy declined sharply among rank-and-file Republicans between 1984 and 2010, while it remained unchanged among Democrats175. Presumably, this reflects economic system justification, that is, the conservative defence of increasing inequality as fair and legitimate under the neoliberal regime101,107,176,177,178,179,180,181. Something similar has transpired with respect to belief in climate change, which has declined among Republicans and self-identified conservatives but not among Democrats and self-identified liberals182,183. Motivated skepticism about anthropogenic climate change is also tied to economic system justification — that is, defence of the capitalist system184,185 — which probably exacerbates climate change polarization as well as partisan and ideological alignment on both economic and environmental issues.

As noted above, ‘us versus them’ dynamics can result in stereotyping and false or exaggerated beliefs about the out-group132,146,147,148,149. In one study, Republicans overestimated the percentage of Democrats who were LGBTQ by 25 points, whereas Democrats overestimated the percentage of Republicans who were evangelicals by 20 points150. By exaggerating the prototypical characteristics of out-group members in these ways, ideologues increase the perceived social distance between different groups and might stereotype, discredit or marginalize the out-group. Partisans might also represent their adversaries as more obstructionist95, more ideologically extreme122,151,152,153, more prejudiced against the in-group55, or more violent154 than they actually are. Presumably, distorting images of the out-group as deviant, hostile, stubborn, extreme and unreasonable serves to justify treating the out-group unfavourably55,89,95,155.

Cowan, S. K. & Baldassarri, D. “It could turn ugly”: selective disclosure of attitudes in political discussion networks. Soc. Netw. 52, 1–17 (2018). Using a novel set of survey questions, this research illustrates the mechanism of selective disclosure: the tendency to withhold political attitudes from those with whom one disagrees in an attempt to avoid conflict.

Klar, S., Krupnikov, Y. & Ryan, J. B. Affective polarization or partisan disdain? Untangling a dislike for the opposing party from a dislike of partisanship. Public. Opin. Q. 82, 379–390 (2018).

Polarization definition psychology

Sorace, M. & Hobolt, S. B. A tale of two peoples: motivated reasoning in the aftermath of the Brexit vote. Polit. Sci. Res. Meth. 9, 675–692 (2021).

Lindaman, K. & Haider-Markel, D. P. Issue evolution, political parties, and the culture wars. Polit. Res. Q. 55, 91–110 (2002).

Monocular eyepiece: This is a traditional way of working with eyepieces, but is now rarely used. It has only one lens and the observer observes the specimen with one eye.

Monocular cylinder: This type of cylinder has become less used. It allows the observer to observe the specimen using only one eye and is less suitable for applications that require in-depth observation and recording of microscopic structures.

Kim, T. Violent political rhetoric on Twitter. Political Sci. Res. Methods. https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2022.12 (2022).

Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. in Cognitive Dissonance: Reexamining a Pivotal Theory in Psychology 3–24 (American Psychological Association, 2019).

Guth, J. L. & Nelsen, B. F. Party choice in Europe: social cleavages and the rise of populist parties. Party Politics 27, 453–464 (2021).

Mullinix, K. J. Partisanship and preference formation: competing motivations, elite polarization, and issue importance. Polit. Behav. 38, 383–411 (2016).

Ang, Z., Reeves, A., Rogowski, J. C. & Vishwanath, A. Partisanship, economic assessments, and presidential accountability. Am. J. Polit. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12659 (2021).

Binocular barrel: Binocular barrel is the common type. It allows both eyes to observe the specimen simultaneously, providing a more natural and comfortable viewing experience. Typically, binocular cartridges have adjustable pupil distance and telescopic range to accommodate different observers.

An important feature of the social-communicative context in which polarization and depolarization take place is the channel (or platform). For instance, participants in the 6 January 2020 insurrection attended an extremely combative, in-person ‘Stop the Steal’ event earlier in the day featuring a dozen live speakers in addition to Donald Trump. Other appeals to engage in polarization or depolarization may come from face-to-face conversations, church sermons, television commentators, newspaper editorials, internet news sources, or social media platforms. Some platforms are more emotionally arousing, while others are more effective in conveying accurate (or inaccurate) information188.

Gidron, N., Adams, J. & Horne, W. American Affective Polarization in Comparative Perspective (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2020).

Imhoff, R. et al. Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 392–403 (2022). This study found that, across 26 countries, rightists scored consistently higher than leftists on a generalized conspiracy mentality scale in 11 countries (Austria, Belgium, Denmark, France, Germany, The Netherlands, Norway, Poland, Portugal, Sweden and Switzerland). In three countries (Hungary, Romania and the UK), there were conflicting results; there was only one country (Spain) where leftists were more conspiracy-minded than rightists.

Hooghe, L. & Marks, G. Cleavage theory meets Europe’s crises: Lipset, Rokkan, and the transnational cleavage. J. Eur. Public Policy 25, 109–135 (2018).

Yarchi, M., Baden, C. & Kligler-Vilenchik, N. Political polarization on the digital sphere: a cross-platform, over-time analysis of interactional, positional, and affective polarization on social media. Polit. Commun. 38, 98–139 (2021).

Ashokkumar, A. et al. Censoring political opposition online: who does it and why. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 91, 104031 (2020). This study finds that supporters of a political cause (such as abortion restriction or gun control) recommend deleting ideologically incongruent messages and banning sources of ideologically incongruent messages, even when messages are inoffensive.

Healthy democratic polities feature competing visions of a good society but also require some level of cooperation and institutional trust. Democracy is at risk when citizens become so polarized that an ‘us versus them’ mentality dominates. Despite a vast multidisciplinary literature, no coherent conceptual framework of the microlevel dynamics that increase or decrease polarization has been presented. In this Review, we provide a conceptual framework to integrate scientific knowledge about cognitive–motivational mechanisms that influence political polarization and the social-communicative contexts in which they are enacted. Ego-justifying and group-justifying motives lead individuals to defend their own pre-existing beliefs and those of their in-group, respectively. However, a distinct class of system-justifying motives contributes to asymmetric forms of polarization. Whereas conservative-rightist ideology is associated with valuing tradition, social order and maintenance of the status quo, liberal-leftist ideology is associated with a push for egalitarian social change. These cognitive–motivational mechanisms interact with social influence processes linked to communication source, message and channel factors, all of which might contribute to increased or decreased polarization. We conclude with a discussion of unanswered questions and ways in which our framework can be extended to the study of culture and institutions.

Franks, A. S. & Hesami, F. Seeking evidence of the MAGA cult and Trump derangement syndrome: an examination of (a)symmetric political bias. Societies 11, 113 (2021).

Reiljan, A. The Politics Of Differentiated Integration: What Do Governments Want? Country Report Research Paper No. RSCAS 2020/92 https://ssrn.com/abstract=3783232 (Estonia Robert Schuman Centre for Advanced Studies, 2020).

Kubin, E., Puryear, C., Schein, C. & Gray, K. Personal experiences bridge moral and political divides better than facts. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2008389118 (2021).

Levy, R. E. Social media, news consumption, and polarization: evidence from a field experiment. Am. Econ. Rev. 111, 831–870 (2021).

Spohr, D. Fake news and ideological polarization: filter bubbles and selective exposure on social media. Bus. Inf. Rev. 34, 150–160 (2017).

Hargittai, E., Gallo, J. & Kane, M. Cross-ideological discussions among conservative and liberal bloggers. Public Choice 134, 67–86 (2008).

Klar, S., & Krupnikov, Y. Independent Politics: How American Disdain For Parties Leads To Political Inaction (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2016).

Huber, M., Van Boven, L., Park, B. & Pizzi, W. T. Seeing red: anger increases how much Republican identification predicts partisan attitudes and perceived polarization. PLoS ONE 10, e0139193 (2015).

Wilson, A. E., Parker, V. & Feinberg, M. Polarization in the contemporary political and media landscape. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 223–228 (2020).

Behavioural norms can also work in subtler ways, without formal communication, as on the basis of common knowledge (such as what one believes to be socially acceptable by others) or fear of being sanctioned for deviating from expected or prescribed behaviour. For example, Republicans were less likely to wear a mask in public during the COVID-19 pandemic if their neighbourhood had a higher (versus lower) proportion of fellow Republicans living in it325. However, the social context did not affect unobservable health behaviours such as vaccination, nor did it affect Democrats’ behaviour with regard to any outcomes, including masking325. Because Democrats were more likely to wear masks in general15, in this case the social norm contributed to asymmetric issue polarization. There is also evidence that people who hold conservative (versus liberal) attitudes are more likely to prioritize values of conformity and tradition; to possess a strong desire to share reality with like-minded others; to perceive greater within-group consensus when making political and non-political judgements; to be influenced by implicit relational cues and sources who are perceived as similar to themselves; and to maintain homogeneous social networks and favour an ‘echo chamber’ environment that is conducive to the spread of misinformation326.

Arceneaux, K. & Johnson, M. Changing Minds or Changing Channels? Partisan News in an Age of Choice (Univ. Chicago Press, 2013).

Rigoli, F. Masters of suspicion: a Bayesian decision model of motivated political reasoning. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 51, 350–370 (2021).

The impact of narratives may depend upon the source, or narrator. In one experiment, for instance, exposure to Joe Biden’s storytelling increased support for social security among Democrats but not Republicans, thereby increasing issue polarization. However, the narrative did increase liking for Biden in all participants291. In general, sharing personal experiences rather than facts appears to foster respect and humanize politicians, even in the eyes of their adversaries, thereby reducing affective polarization292,297. Narrative techniques thus hold promise for bringing people together, but their effectiveness might raise other concerns. Personal stories can be manufactured or manipulated and are difficult to verify; to the extent that they are more influential than accurate information, they can be used to mislead followers and sow discord298.

Grossmann, M. & Thaler, D. Mass–elite divides in aversion to social change and support for Donald Trump. Am. Polit. Res. 46, 753–784 (2018).

Cialdini, R. B. & Jacobson, R. P. Influences of social norms on climate change-related behaviors. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 42, 1–8 (2021).

Knuckey, J. & Hassan, K. Authoritarianism and support for Trump in the 2016 presidential election. Soc. Sci. J. 59, 47–60 (2022).

Fernbach, P. M. & Boven, L. V. False polarization: cognitive mechanisms and potential solutions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 43, 1–6 (2022). This essay addresses how false polarization — where partisans hold false beliefs about the other side — can lead to actual polarization owing to categorical thinking, oversimplification and emotional amplification.

Kalla, J. L. & Broockman, D. E. Reducing exclusionary attitudes through interpersonal conversation: evidence from three field experiments. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 114, 410–425 (2020).

Lau, R. R., Anderson, D. J., Ditono, T. M., Kleinberg, M. S. & Redlawsk, D. P. Effect of media environment diversity and advertising tone on information search, selective exposure, and affective polarization. Polit. Behav. 39, 231–255 (2017).

Molden, D. C., Bayes, R. & Druckman, J. N. A motivational systems approach to investigating opinions on climate change. Think. Reason. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2021.1982003 (2021).

Much has been written about how Internet and social media platforms exacerbate political polarization70,127,231,255,256,257,258,259,260,261,262,263. Personalized machine-learning algorithms and the freedom to choose content tend to increase the likelihood of selective information exposure to previously unimaginable levels. Thus, many worry that social media platforms are merely becoming ideological echo chambers in which people who are strongly motivated by ego-justifying, group-justifying or system-justifying (or system-challenging) goals seek out like-minded others261,264,265. At the same time, it is important to bear in mind that patterns of online ideological segregation resemble offline patterns of media consumption205, and most social media users are exposed to reasonable levels of ideological heterogeneity206,207,257,266,267,268.

Jacoby, W. G. Is there a culture war? Conflicting value structures in American public opinion. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 108, 754–771 (2014).

Elites are not the only source of potentially polarizing or depolarizing communications. Social networks comprised of friends, family members and co-workers also influence individuals’ political attitudes and behaviour23,200,201,202. Whether offline202,203,204 or online205,206,207, people tend to share information and discuss politics with like-minded others, but they are seldom completely cut off from disagreement208.

Broockman, D. E. & Kalla, J. L. Durably reducing transphobia: a field experiment on door-to-door canvassing. Science 352, 220–224 (2016).

Rogers, N. & Jost, J. T. Liberals as cultural omnivores. J. Assoc. Consum. Res. 7, 255–265 (2022). This analysis reveals that self-identified liberalism was positively associated with the total number of cultural exposures across a wide range of domains. The ideological asymmetry in cultural sorting was statistically mediated by individual differences in openness to new experiences.

Experimental approaches have been especially useful for documenting the polarizing effects of different types of social media usage. In one study, a randomly assigned group of Facebook users deactivated Facebook for four weeks prior to the 2018 USA midterm election. When compared to a matched control group, deactivation reduced issue polarization, and to a lesser extent, affective polarization271. These effects might be due in part to the fact that deactivating social media accounts decreases exposure to fake news256.

Martinez, J. E., Feldman, L. A., Feldman, M. J. & Cikara, M. Narratives shape cognitive representations of immigrants and immigration-policy preferences. Psychol. Sci. 32, 135–152 (2021).

Berman, C. When revolutionary coalitions break down: polarization, protest, and the Tunisian political crisis of August 2013. Middle East Law Gov. 11, 136–179 (2019).

Lord, C. G., Ross, L. & Lepper, M. R. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: the effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 2098–2109 (1979).

Social psychologists have long argued that people are driven to maintain or enhance cognitive consistency and congruence among beliefs, opinions and values108,109. Consistent with this argument, there is abundant evidence that since the 1960s those who endorse liberal attitudes have increasingly joined the Democratic Party, and those who endorse conservative attitudes have increasingly joined the Republican Party35,110. Consequently, the correlation between partisanship and ideology has risen from roughly r = 0.4 in 1988 to r = 0.6 in 2012 (refs.47,91). From a psychological perspective, the increasing alignment between relational (or identity-based) and ideological commitments could reflect a reduction in cognitive dissonance. That is, to maintain the integrity of their self-concept, people seek to reduce inconsistencies between self-categorization as a good group member, on one hand, and important beliefs, opinions and values, on the other. From a more macro-political perspective, increased alignment between partisanship and ideology reflects a ‘sorting’ process that increases intergroup conflict35,38,77.

Triocular tube: Triocular tube is commonly used for photomicrography needs. It allows the observer to view the specimen using both eyes, while also having an additional optical channel that can be connected to a camera device to take high-quality micrographs and videos.

Image

Blatz, C. W. & Mercier, B. False polarization and false moderation: political opponents overestimate the extremity of each other’s ideologies but underestimate each other’s certainty. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 9, 521–529 (2018).

Jenkins-Smith, H. C. et al. Partisan asymmetry in temporal stability of climate change beliefs. Nat. Clim. Change 10, 322–328 (2020).

Kligler-Vilenchik, N., Baden, C. & Yarchi, M. Interpretative polarization across platforms: how political disagreement develops over time on Facebook, Twitter, and WhatsApp. Soc. Media Soc. 6, 2056305120944393 (2020).

For political polarization (or depolarization) to occur, the cognitive–motivational mechanisms described above must play out in some social or political context61. To systematize our discussion of social-communicative contexts we invoke McGuire’s188 communication/persuasion matrix, which distinguishes among source, message, channel, receiver and target (or destination) characteristics as determinants of social influence20,189,190. A guiding assumption of this general approach is that the kinds of cognitive and motivational mechanisms described above play mediating (or, in some cases, moderating) roles in explaining, for instance, whether specific messages communicated by certain sources are persuasive — or, in the present context, serve to increase or decrease political polarization.

Dias, N. & Lelkes, Y. The nature of affective polarization: disentangling policy disagreement from partisan identity. Am. J. Polit. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12628 (2021).

Neal, Z. P. A sign of the times? Weak and strong polarization in the US Congress, 1973–2016. Soc. Netw. 60, 103–112 (2020).

Ditto, P. H. et al. At least bias is bipartisan: a meta-analytic comparison of partisan bias in liberals and conservatives. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 273–291 (2019).

Pronin, E., Lin, D. Y. & Ross, L. The bias blind spot: perceptions of bias in self versus others. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 369–381 (2002).

On 6 January 2021, thousands of supporters of President Donald Trump stormed the US Capitol in an effort to overturn his defeat in the 2020 presidential election. For years, pundits and observers had predicted that violence and democratic breakdown would inevitably result from the rise in polarization, ‘tribal politics’ and increasing hostility between those who hold liberal attitudes and those who hold conservative attitudes1,2,3. Even in the aftermath of the ‘insurrection’, which injured 138 police officers and killed five people, the American public remained deeply divided. Democrats felt the rioters should be prosecuted, whereas Republicans were ambivalent, with half of them stating that the riot received too much attention4.

Womick, J., Rothmund, T., Azevedo, F., King, L. A. & Jost, J. T. Group-based dominance and authoritarian aggression predict support for Donald Trump in the 2016 US presidential election. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 10, 643–652 (2019).

Morisi, D., Jost, J. T., Panagopoulos, C., & Valtonen, J. Is there an ideological asymmetry in the incumbency effect? Evidence from US Congressional elections. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506211046830 (2022).

Leeper, T. J. & Slothuus, R. Political parties, motivated reasoning, and public opinion formation. Polit. Psychol. 35, 129–156 (2014).

Lees, J. & Cikara, M. Understanding and combating misperceived polarization. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200143 (2021). This article reviews work on false polarization (when partisans hold inaccurate beliefs about the other side), identifies conditions false polarization leads to actual polarization, and suggests why correcting perceptions about the other party’s beliefs can be effective.

Luttig, M. The structure of inequality and Americans’ attitudes toward redistribution. Public. Opin. Q. 77, 811–821 (2013).

Lelkes, Y. Affective polarization and ideological sorting: a reciprocal, albeit weak, relationship. Forum 16, 67–79 (2018).

Abramowitz, A. I. The Great Alignment: Race, Party Transformation, and the Rise of Donald Trump (Yale Univ. Press, 2018).

Pew Research Center. Large majority of the public views prosecution of capitol rioters as ‘very important’. Pew Research https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/2021/03/18/large-majority-of-the-public-views-prosecution-of-capitol-rioters-as-very-important/ (2021).

Message framing has the potential to reduce polarization to the extent that it assuages ego- and group-justification motives by, for example, alleviating personal or collective insecurity, or taps into system-justification motives by appealing to patriotic traditions. However, some frames may increase polarization by eliciting ego- or group-defensiveness, accentuating categorical differences between groups, or highlighting individual and group differences in system-justifying (or system-challenging) attitudes. Unfortunately, there are often political incentives for partisan elites to exploit the more polarizing strategies of message framing to maximize support and turnout among their staunchest constituents.

Westfall, J., Van Boven, L., Chambers, J. R. & Judd, C. M. Perceiving political polarization in the United States: party identity strength and attitude extremity exacerbate the perceived partisan divide. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 145–158 (2015).

Stone, J., Whitehead, J., Schmader, T. & Focella, E. Thanks for asking: self-affirming questions reduce backlash when stigmatized targets confront prejudice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 589–598 (2011).

Ahler, D. J. & Broockman, D. E. The delegate paradox: why polarized politicians can represent citizens best. J. Polit. 80, 1117–1133 (2018).

The twenty-first century has brought with it a remarkable transformation of political life. Today more than half of Americans obtain political news from sources that either did not exist (such as social media) or were still emerging (cable news networks) twenty years ago316. Norms of engagement are still evolving and have yet to reach a stable equilibrium. A major challenge for future research is to identify ways in which partisans and ideologues can engage with one another constructively317, rather than trafficking in hatred and the politics of provocation and backlash127,258. The latter style breeds political disengagement244, placing the fate of democracy in the hands of a small, non-representative subset of citizens. Understanding and overcoming these dangerous dynamics requires that scholars and social scientists recognize the full range of cognitive–motivational mechanisms and social-communicative contexts that drive human behaviour in a world in which politics, for better or worse, plays an ever-greater part in so many facets of public and private life107.

Image

In summary, ideological and issue polarization, partisan alignment, and affective polarization (like all other outcomes of social influence) depend on who, what and how, that is, the source of communication, the particulars of the message and the channel or platform of transmission20,188,189,190. Some factors increase polarization, as when highly polarized elites stoke issue, ideological and affective polarization among voters, or when restrictive framing of nationalist messages increases partisan or ideological alignment with respect to immigration or foreign policy. Other variables contribute to depolarization, as when strategic framing of environmental values, activation of system justification motives at the national level, and sympathetic narratives about immigrants bring liberals and conservatives closer together (at least temporarily).

Mellers, B., Tetlock, P. & Arkes, H. R. Forecasting tournaments, epistemic humility and attitude depolarization. Cognition 188, 19–26 (2019).

Druckman, J. N., Klar, S., Krupnikov, Y., Levendusky, M. & Ryan, J. B. (Mis-) estimating affective polarization. J. Polit. 84, 1106–1117 (2022).

Harteveld, E., Mendoza, P. & Rooduijn, M. Affective polarization and the populist radical right: creating the hating? Gov. Oppos. https://doi.org/10.1017/gov.2021.31 (2021).

Lees, J. & Cikara, M. Inaccurate group meta-perceptions drive negative out-group attributions in competitive contexts. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 279–286 (2020).

Frames can also shift perceptions of the social system and therefore the basis for system justification. For example, describing the USA as a ‘nation of immigrants’ would have very different implications for ideology and public opinion than describing it as a white-majority country or one ‘settled’ by Europeans. Different ideological groups are likely to defend and justify different aspects of the overarching social system. For instance, in 2016, Donald Trump supporters were more likely to justify economic inequality under capitalism and gender disparities under the patriarchal social order, in comparison with supporters of Hillary Clinton, but they were not more likely to justify the general social system in the USA following eight years of Barack Obama’s presidency284.

Jacquet, J., Dietrich, M. & Jost, J. T. The ideological divide and climate change opinion: “top-down” and “bottom-up” approaches. Front. Psychol. 5, 1458 (2014).

Van Bavel, J. J., Rathje, S., Harris, E., Robertson, C. & Sternisko, A. How social media shapes polarization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 913–916 (2021). This review suggests that social media platforms exacerbate political polarization through mechanisms of partisan selection, message content, platform design and computerized algorithms.

a | High issue/ideological polarization occurs when one (asymmetric) or both groups (symmetric) move towards the extremes and away from the centre with respect to issues and/or ideology. There is low issue/ideological polarization when most people hold moderate positions and there is considerable overlap between groups. b | Partisan issue alignment occurs when groups neatly divide over many issues. Low alignment occurs when the issue preferences of various segments of society are crosscutting. c | Affective polarization occurs when members of different groups (or parties) hold starkly positive feelings about members of their own group and/or starkly negative feelings about members of the other group(s).

Mernyk, J. S., Pink, S. L., Druckman, J. N. & Willer, R. Correcting inaccurate metaperceptions reduces Americans’ support for partisan violence. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2116851119 (2022).

Mikołajczak, G., Becker, J. C. & Iyer, A. Women who challenge or defend the status quo: ingroup identities as predictors of progressive and reactionary collective action. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2842 (2022).

Some Americans have ‘tuned out’ of politics in recent years243,244, which might suggest that the effect of the mass media on polarization has dwindled240,242. However, polarization effects can still spread through a two-step communication process whereby individuals who have been exposed to partisan media influence co-partisans who have not been directly exposed (Fig. 2), thereby increasing overall levels of attitudinal extremity253.

Rollwage, M., Zmigrod, L., de-Wit, L., Dolan, R. J. & Fleming, S. M. What underlies political polarization? A manifesto for computational political psychology. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 820–822 (2019).

Kalla, J. L. & Broockman, D. E. Which narrative strategies durably reduce prejudice? Evidence from field and survey experiments supporting the efficacy of perspective-getting. Am. J. Polit. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12657 (2021).

Schwalbe, M. C., Cohen, G. L. & Ross, L. D. The objectivity illusion and voter polarization in the 2016 presidential election. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 21218–21229 (2020).

Framing occurs whenever a political actor (such as a candidate or opinion leader) highlights a subset of relevant considerations about an issue, candidate or event, leading their audience members to think about the topic in a particular way273,274,275. Several studies show that stressing certain values can change support for specific issues. For example, framing environmental issues in terms of conservative values such as purity, sanctity, commerce or patriotism might lead conservatives to express more support for pro-environmental legislation275,276,277,278,279. Abstract rhetoric used by in-group members may also increase alignment between values and identities. For instance, Republican elite discourse has for decades emphasized traditional family values and resistance to social change, and this seems to have increased the correlation between partisanship and ideology more steeply among Republican than Democratic voters47.

Bar-Tal, D. Self-censorship as a socio-political-psychological phenomenon: conception and research. Polit. Psychol. 38 (suppl. 1), 37–65 (2017).

Matz, S. C. Personal echo chambers: openness-to-experience is linked to higher levels of psychological interest diversity in large-scale behavioral data. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 121, 1284–1300 (2021).

These and other studies327,328 highlight the ways in which norms tied to social contexts such as geographic locations influence political preferences and contribute to polarization329. Whether political ideology and partisanship drive choices about where to reside remains a subject of debate209,210. In any case, many partisans in the USA appear to live in areas where they have little exposure to out-party members211,330. In addition, over the past three decades, Americans’ personal networks have become smaller and more homogeneous in terms of political preferences, apparently because ‘important matters’ are increasingly framed as ideologically meaningful204. These developments have the capacity to exacerbate all three types of polarization, insofar as individuals bring their own political attitudes and beliefs into alignment with those who are spatially proximate329,331 and tend to form negative impressions, including misperceptions, about those with whom they rarely interact332.

The source of communication and/or persuasion is the person or group who is potentially capable of influencing others, whether they intend to or not. It can refer to elites, such as politicians and journalists, or to peers, such as friends or family members. Sources are more effective in influencing others when they are perceived as in-group members92 and as credible, trustworthy, powerful and/or attractive188.

Finkel, E. J. et al. Political sectarianism in America. Science 370, 533–536 (2020). This article discusses the causes and consequences of a concept related to affective polarization — political sectarianism — which involves othering, aversion and moralization.

Jost, J. T. et al. How social media facilitates political protest: information, motivation, and social networks. Polit. Psychol. 39 (suppl. 1), 58–118 (2018).

Lelkes, Y., Sood, G. & Iyengar, S. The hostile audience: the effect of access to broadband internet on partisan affect. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 61, 5–20 (2017).

Webber, D., Kruglanski, A., Molinario, E. & Jasko, K. Ideologies that justify political violence. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 107–111 (2020).

Gerber, A., Huber, G., Doherty, D. & Dowling, C. Disagreement and the avoidance of political discussion: aggregate relationships and differences across personality traits. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 56, 849–874 (2012).

Because issue and affective polarization have increased most among demographic groups (such as the elderly) that are less likely to use the internet and social media platforms, the authors of one study272 concluded that social media was unlikely to be a major driver of polarization. However, this conclusion was based on a design in which individual social media usage was not measured or manipulated, and so could be subject to the ecological fallacy (drawing inappropriate inferences about individual behavior on the basis of aggregate trends261).

Lang, J., Erickson, W. W. & Jing-Schmidt, Z. #MaskOn! #MaskOff! Digital polarization of mask-wearing in the United States during COVID-19. PLoS ONE 16, e0250817 (2021).

Bougher, L. D. The correlates of discord: identity, issue alignment, and political hostility in polarized America. Polit. Behav. 39, 731–762 (2017).

DiMaggio, P., Evans, J. & Bryson, B. Have Americans’ social attitudes become more polarized? Am. J. Sociol. 102, 690–755 (1996).

Tappin, B. M., Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Thinking clearly about causal inferences of politically motivated reasoning: why paradigmatic study designs often undermine causal inference. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 81–87 (2020).

Druckman, J. N., Gubitz, S. R., Levendusky, M. S. & Lloyd, A. How incivility on partisan media (de-)polarizes the electorate. J. Polit. 81, 291–295 (2019).

Archival research reveals that mainstream media coverage of Democratic elites’ support for climate action stimulated resistance and even backlash (decreased support) among Republican voters144. Likewise, exposure to same-party and other-party messages on partisan media platforms often increases affective polarization, in the latter case because of resistance to change and the spontaneous generation of counterarguments249. Thus, out-party cues often repel citizens while in-party cues tend to attract them. In this way, ego- and group-justifying motives exacerbate issue polarization when viewers are aware of partisan or ideological disagreement. Such circumstances also make individual and group differences in system justification motivation more predictive of political behavior and more salient to political actors by drawing attention to the fact that some people are driven to defend the status quo while others are driven to challenge it101,107,171,172,250,251,252.

Van Boven, L., Ehret, P. J. & Sherman, D. K. Psychological barriers to bipartisan public support for climate policy. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 492–507 (2018).

Iyengar, S. & Westwood, S. J. Fear and loathing across party lines: new evidence on group polarization. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 59, 690–707 (2015).

Van Bavel, J. J., Reinero, D. A., Spring, V., Harris, E. & Duke, A. Speaking my truth: why personal experiences can bridge divides but mislead. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2100280118 (2021).

Krupnikov, Y. & Ryan J. B. The Other Divide: Polarization And Disengagement In American Politics (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2022).

Descriptive social norms about what other people think or do and injunctive norms about what people ought to think or do are two major sources of social influence322. Both can lead partisans to feel socially pressured to hold particular opinions or enact certain behaviours. In one experiment, Republicans received a message indicating that a clear majority of fellow Republicans agreed that the climate is changing and are taking action to combat climate change. These individuals — relative to those who did not receive this message — were more likely to report believing in climate change and to have climate-friendly intentions323,324. In this case, information about descriptive norms served to reduce issue polarization concerning climate change, but the effects generally depend upon the message and topic.

Draca, M. & Schwarz, C. How polarized are citizens? Measuring ideology from the ground-up. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3154431 (2021).

It is especially important to distinguish between states and trends when studying polarization in cross-national contexts. For example, there is evidence that ideological, issue and affective polarization have increased rapidly in the USA in the past few decades339,340, but it is also true that levels of polarization in the USA are smaller than (or similar to) levels observed in other countries65,341. Historically speaking, polarization in the USA reflects social patterns that began with Southern realignment following civil rights legislation, the rise of single-issue interest groups, changes in campaign finance law, and the withdrawal of moderates from party politics and primary races, especially on the political right10,27,31,34,37,72,342,343). By contrast, the rise of polarization in Europe is largely attributed to anti-establishment movements and newly founded populist parties67,344,345.

Mason, L. “I disrespectfully agree”: the differential effects of partisan sorting on social and issue polarization. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 59, 128–145 (2015).

Achen, C. H. & Bartels, L. M. Democracy For Realists: Why Elections Do Not Produce Responsive Government (Princeton Univ. Press, 2016).

Brewer, M. B. In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: a cognitive-motivational analysis. Psychol. Bull. 86, 307 (1979).

In the complex world of microscopy, the cylinder and eyepiece are difficult or absent elements that work together to enable us to observe tiny biological and material structures in depth. This article will introduce the functions and types of eyepiece and cylinder, and their role in the microscope.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

Sidanius, J. & Liu, J. H. The Gulf War and the Rodney King beating: implications of the general conservatism and social dominance perspectives. J. Soc. Psychol. 132, 685–700 (1992).

Kobayashi, T. & Katagiri, A. The “rally around the flag” effect in territorial disputes: experimental evidence from Japan–China relations. J. East Asian Stud. 18, 299–319 (2018).

Hart, P. S., Feldman, L., Leiserowitz, A. & Maibach, E. Extending the impacts of hostile media perceptions: influences on discussion and opinion polarization in the context of climate change. Sci. Commun. 37, 506–532 (2015).

Baron, J. & Jost, J. T. False equivalence: are liberals and conservatives in the United States equally biased? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 292–303 (2019).

Polarization effects may spread through a two-step communication process whereby individuals who have been exposed to partisan media influence co-partisans (and others) who have not been directly exposed.

Trump, K. S. Income inequality influences perceptions of legitimate income differences. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 48, 929–952 (2018).

Hobolt, S. B., Leeper, T. J. & Tilley, J. Divided by the vote: affective polarization in the wake of the Brexit referendum. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 51, 1476–1493 (2021).

Polarization in Physics

Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L. & Kafati, G. Group identity and intergroup relations: the common in-group identity model. Adv. Group. Process. 17, 1–35 (2000).

Rossiter, E. The consequences of interparty conversation on outparty affect and stereotypes. Preprint at http://erossiter.com/files/conversations.pdf (2020).

Finally, in-group favouritism, out-group derogation and refusal to compromise seem to be more strongly associated with conservative ideology and Republican identification in the USA than with liberal ideology and Democratic identification10,73,74,75. In line with this asymmetry, self-identified conservatives are more likely than liberals to exhibit the aforementioned ‘President-in-power’ effect; that is, to trust the government more when their own party (versus the other party) holds the White House161,186,187. These findings have clear implications for asymmetric polarization: in game-theoretic terms, people who embrace liberal ideology are more likely than those who embrace conservative ideology to ‘cooperate’, whereas the latter are more likely to ‘defect’107.

Message factors refer to the style and content of a communication, such as rhetorical and other characteristics of a message that make it more or less likely to produce polarization or depolarization. For example, some appeals are based on emotion, while others are based on logic; still others construct compelling narratives based on single events or familiar tropes, and political actors engaged in social influence attempts often frame their arguments strategically or opportunistically to increase the likelihood of achieving their political goals.

Bonikowski, B., Feinstein, Y. & Bock, S. The partisan sorting of “America”: How nationalist cleavages shaped the 2016 US Presidential election. Am. J. Sociol. 127, 492–561 (2021).

Layman, G. C., Carsey, T. M. & Horowitz, J. M. Party polarization in American politics: characteristics, causes, and consequences. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 9, 83–110 (2006).

Bond, R. M. et al. A 61-million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization. Nature 489, 295–298 (2012).

Conover, M. D., Gonçalves, B., Flammini, A. & Menczer, F. Partisan asymmetries in online political activity. EPJ Data Sci. 1, 6 (2012).

Brewer, M. B. in The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice (eds Sibley, C. G. & Barlow, F. K.) 90–110 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2017).

van Baar, J. M. & FeldmanHall, O. The polarized mind in context: interdisciplinary approaches to the psychology of political polarization. Am. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000814 (2022).

Zmigrod, L. The role of cognitive rigidity in political ideologies: theory, evidence, and future directions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 34–39 (2020).

Azevedo, F. & Jost, J. T. The ideological basis of anti-scientific attitudes: effects of authoritarianism, conservatism, religiosity, social dominance, and system justification. Group Process. Interg. Relat. 24, 518–549 (2021).

Voelkel, J. G. et al. Interventions reducing affective polarization do not improve anti-democratic attitudes. Nat. Hum. Behav. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/7evmp (2021).

Boxell, L., Gentzkow, M. & Shapiro, J. Cross-country trends in affective polarization. Rev. Econ. Stat. https://doi.org/10.1162/rest_a_01160 (2022).

Fifth, although we began this Review by mentioning the Capitol insurrection on 6 January 2021, the precise connections among various forms of polarization, abandonment of democratic norms and principles, and political violence are unknown. Research to date paints a confusing picture, with some studies indicating a negative relationship between affective polarization and support for democratic norms302,303, and others finding no connection between the two13,58. Moreover, the literature on political violence304 is largely disconnected from research on polarization. Understanding how polarization affects other political outcomes, including regime failure, is especially important for designing policy interventions305. One important question is whether depolarization efforts should be holistic or targeted at specific groups, such as right-wing extremists306,307,308,309.

The cylinder and eyepiece are important parts of the microscope, and their design and quality directly affect the observation effect and comfort. Binocular cartridges and high-quality eyepieces allow the observer to view the specimen more easily, reduce eye strain, and obtain higher quality microscopic images. At the same time, the triocular tube facilitates microphotography, allowing scientists to record and share their findings.

Gollwitzer, A. et al. Partisan differences in physical distancing are linked to health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 1186–1197 (2020). This analysis of geotracking data of 15 million smartphones per day indicated that people in conservative and Republican (versus liberal and Democratic) counties exhibited 14% less physical distancing in the early months of the COVID-19 pandemic, and partisan and ideological differences in physical distancing predicted higher infection rates and increased mortality in pro-Trump counties.

Druckman, J. N., Levendusky, M. S. & McLain, A. No need to watch: how the effects of partisan media can spread via inter-personal discussions. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 62, 99–112 (2018).

Baldassarri, D. & Page, S. E. The emergence and perils of polarization. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2116863118 (2021).

Bowes, S. M., Blanchard, M. C., Costello, T. H., Abramowitz, A. I. & Lilienfeld, S. O. Intellectual humility and between-party animus: implications for affective polarization in two community samples. J. Res. Pers. 88, 103992 (2020).

Brown, J. R. & Enos, R. D. The measurement of partisan sorting for 180 million voters. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 998–1008 (2021).

Lee, S., Rojas, H. & Yamamoto, M. Social media, messaging apps, and affective polarization in the United States and Japan. Mass. Commun. Soc. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2021.1953534 (2021).

Additional cognitive–motivational mechanisms that may exacerbate issue polarization include other forms of reduction in cognitive dissonance, such as selective avoidance of counter-attitudinal information and motivated resistance to attitude change (see Table 1), both of which reflect ego-defensive motives to avoid inconsistency and maintain the individual’s pre-existing beliefs, opinions and values100,123,124,125,126. For instance, Republicans who were consistently exposed to messages from a liberal Twitter bot exhibited backlash, expressing even more conservative opinions as a result127. This suggests the presence of ego-defensiveness: Republicans were not merely indifferent to liberal messaging but actively sought to combat it or to compensate for its anticipated effects. Exposure to a conservative bot did not meaningfully affect Democrats in this study. Other studies suggest that people might engage in counterfactual thinking about ‘what might have been’ in a creative but self-serving (or group-serving) manner in order to maintain their own pre-existing attitudes. For example, Donald Trump supporters felt that it was less unethical to spread the falsehood that more people attended Trump’s presidential inauguration than Barack Obama’s presidential inauguration after pondering the counterfactual possibility that “If security had been less tight at Trump’s inauguration, then more people would have attended it than Obama’s inauguration,” compared to an experimental condition in which this counterfactual was not raised128. Thus, people may rationalize the spread of misinformation through the biased use of information about ‘what might have been’.

Guess, A. & Coppock, A. Does counter-attitudinal information cause backlash? Results from three large survey experiments. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 50, 1497–1515 (2020).

Isom, D. A., Mikell, T. C. & Boehme, H. M. White America, threat to the status quo, and affiliation with the alt-right: a qualitative approach. Sociol. Spect. 41, 213–228 (2021).

Levendusky, M. & Malhotra, N. (Mis)perceptions of partisan polarization in the American public. Public Opin. Q. 80, 378–391 (2016).

Kinder, D. R. & Kalmoe, N. P. Neither Liberal Nor Conservative: Ideological Innocence In The American Public (Univ. Chicago Press, 2017).

Mitchell, A., Jurkowitz, M., Oliphant, J. B. & Shearer, E. Americans who mainly get their news on social media are less engaged, less knowledgeable. Pew Research Center https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2020/07/30/americans-who-mainly-get-their-news-on-social-media-are-less-engaged-less-knowledgeable/ (2020).

Fiorina, M. P., Abrams, S. J. & Pope, J. C. Culture War? The Myth of a Polarized America 3rd edn (Pearson Longman, 2010).

Druckman, J. N., Klar, S., Krupnikov, Y., Levendusky, M. & Ryan, J. B. Affective polarization, local contexts and public opinion in America. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 28–38 (2021). This study shows that levels of partisan animosity measured before the COVID-19 pandemic affected policy beliefs during the pandemic, revealing that affective polarization can influence issue polarization.

Van Assche, J., Dhont, K. & Pettigrew, T. F. The social-psychological bases of far-right support in Europe and the United States. J. Commun. Appl. Soc. 29, 385–401 (2019).

In complex societies, sorting along socio-demographic characteristics is unlikely to bring about complete ideological isolation. This is because most people have crosscutting identities. There are, for instance, wealthy, nonreligious progressive urbanites, as well as morally conservative, low-income ethnic minorities, and both are likely to encounter people who share some but not all of these characteristics, making it difficult for them to inhabit ideological bubbles222. If political considerations become the primary factor driving social relationships, however, people will become much more isolated and polarized along ideological lines59. This is because possessing ideologically homogeneous networks exacerbates issue and affective polarization, whereas having a heterogeneous network facilitates the correction of stereotypical misconceptions and decreases polarization223,224,225,226,227.

Hetherington, M. & Weiler, J. Prius Or Pickup? How The Answers To Four Simple Questions Explain America’s Great Divide (Houghton Mifflin, 2018).

In this Review we have imposed conceptual structure on a vast and rapidly growing but fragmented, multi-disciplinary body of scholarship on political polarization. Specifically, we reviewed evidence concerning the effects of cognitive–motivational mechanisms (ego-justification, group-justification and system-justification) in social-communicative contexts (as a function of source, channel and message factors) on three different operationalizations of political polarization (ideological/issue separation, partisan alignment and affective polarization). Unfortunately, the empirical literature has not advanced to the point that it is possible to hypothesize with precision how any given attempt at political communication will interact with cognitive–motivational mechanisms in specific social-communication contexts to influence polarization or depolarization. We have nonetheless identified the key variables that scholars may use in determining when various types of polarization are likely to arise and — if they deem the situation problematic — in devising depolarization interventions.

Baxter-King, R., Brown, J. R., Enos, R. D., Naeim, A. & Vavreck, L. How local partisan context conditions prosocial behaviors: mask wearing during COVID-19. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2116311119 (2022).

Enders, A. M. & Armaly, M. T. The differential effects of actual and perceived polarization. Polit. Behav. 41, 815–839 (2019).

Westwood, S. J. & Peterson, E. The inseparability of race and partisanship in the United States. Political Behav. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09648-9 (2020).

Plane of polarization

Stern, C. & Crawford, J. T. Ideological conflict and prejudice: an adversarial collaboration examining correlates and ideological (a)symmetries. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 12, 42–53 (2021).

In another experiment, thousands of Facebook users were randomly assigned to subscribe to liberal or conservative news outlets for at least two weeks259. Results of the study confirmed that Facebook’s algorithm limits exposure to counter-attitudinal news in general, but when participants were assigned to receive counter-attitudinal news they were willing to read and share it. Although exposure to counter-attitudinal news did not change participants’ political opinions, it did reduce affective polarization (compared to a control group), possibly because instructions to participants stressed that new and valuable perspectives would be presented, which might have quelled ego-justifying, group-justifying and system-justifying motives.

These findings suggest that the mass polity might in fact be less polarized than people often assume132. Unfortunately, however, even ‘false polarization’ — which may result from the mechanism of social projection, whereby extremists see others as more extreme than they actually are — might become a self-fulfilling prophecy. That is, false polarization might increase affective and/or ideological polarization over time156 because prejudice can arise from even the illusory perception of dissimilarity between in-group and out-group members89,155,157. By contrast, media coverage that is critical of polarization among political elites can reduce issue polarization among citizens, presumably because people do not want to be seen (by themselves or others) as extremists122,158,159,160. Many people, it seems, are amenable to having their misperceptions of the other side corrected, which lowers affective polarization150,153,154.

These observations about the ways in which different types of political polarization amplify others are consistent with social identity theory, which emphasizes the tendency for people to sort themselves into distinct groups that compete for symbolic and material resources88,89,90,91,92. Once categorical boundaries between ‘us and them’ are drawn, a whole host of destructive social psychological processes may kick in, including stereotyping, prejudice, in-group favouritism, out-group derogation and even dehumanization3,55,93,94,95.

Westwood, S. J. et al. The tie that divides: cross-national evidence of the primacy of partyism. Eur. J. Polit. Res. 57, 333–354 (2018).

Druckman, J. N. A framework for the study of persuasion. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 25, 65–88 (2022). This review provides a framework for drawing generalization from research on persuasion, focusing on the actors (speakers and receivers), treatments (topics, content and media), outcomes (attitudes, behaviours, emotions and identities) and settings (competition, space, time, process and culture).

Fourth, our psychological framework cannot easily explain historically changing levels of polarization or elites’ choice of communication strategies. This would require incorporating structural and institutional factors, such as economic performance, social or cultural cleavages across time, and variation in political systems. For example, the Republican Party’s use of restrictive nationalist rhetoric in the twenty-first century286 is almost certainly linked to demographic shifts (such as the relative increase in the non-white population63) in the context of a plurality electoral system with two major parties. It is, in principle, possible to layer structural and institutional variables onto our psychological framework, and this would facilitate comparative analysis of political polarization (Box 2).

Levendusky, M. & Stecula, D. We Need to Talk: How Cross-Party Dialogue Reduces Affective Polarization (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2022).

Tappin, B. M., Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Bayesian or biased? Analytic thinking and political belief updating. Cognition 204, 104375 (2020).

Hennes, E. P., Ruisch, B. C., Feygina, I., Monteiro, C. A. & Jost, J. T. Motivated recall in the service of the economic system: the case of anthropogenic climate change. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 755–771 (2016).

Baker, J. O. & Edmonds, A. E. Immigration, presidential politics, and partisan polarization among the American public, 1992–2018. Sociol. Spectr. 41, 287–303 (2021).

Johnston, R., Manley, D., Jones, K. & Rohla, R. The geographical polarization of the American electorate: a country of increasing electoral landslides? GeoJournal. 85, 187–204 (2020).

Bayes, R., Druckman, J., Goods, A. & Molden, D. C. When and how different motives can drive motivated political reasoning. Polit. Psychol. 41, 1031–1052 (2020).

The desire for cognitive consistency (or belief congruence) also contributes to a wide variety of motivated reasoning processes in politics102. These include confirmation bias, in which people selectively seek out and attend to information that coheres with a desired conclusion (such as their pre-existing belief); biased assimilation, in which people assess the quality of new information based on whether it contradicts or supports a desired conclusion; and disconfirmation bias, in which people place greater scrutiny on, or actively generate counterarguments against, information that undermines a desired conclusion (Table 1). All of these mechanisms, which are sometimes grouped under the general rubric of ‘myside bias’111,112,113, can contribute to issue polarization, insofar as individuals often become more extreme in their opinions after selectively processing the evidence on specific topics such as capital punishment, immigration, healthcare policy or climate change114,115. The precise nature and extent of motivated reasoning on the political left and right is a topic of ongoing research103,111,116,117,118,119.

Bosco, A. & Verney, S. Polarization in southern Europe: elites, party conflicts and negative partisanship. South. Eur. Soc. Politics 25, 257–284 (2020).

Carmines, E. G. & Woods, J. The role of party activists in the evolution of the abortion issue. Polit. Behav. 24, 361–377 (2002).

Jost, J.T., Baldassarri, D.S. & Druckman, J.N. Cognitive–motivational mechanisms of political polarization in social-communicative contexts. Nat Rev Psychol 1, 560–576 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00093-5

Feinberg, M. & Willer, R. Moral reframing: a technique for effective and persuasive communication across political divides. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass. 13, e12501 (2019).

The eyepiece is usually located at the top of the microscope, and its role is to further enlarge the image after the objective lens is enlarged, so that the human eye can clearly observe the specimen. The eyepiece is divided into two working modes: monocular and binocular.

Stronger evidence that affective polarization can generate issue polarization comes from a study demonstrating that out-party animus measured prior to the COVID-19 outbreak was a strong predictor of issue polarization concerning ‘stay-at-home’ orders and other pandemic-related policies, even after adjusting for partisanship15. Furthermore, partisanship exacerbates issue-based conflict27, and party activists are capable of pushing candidates to take more extreme positions on issues over time37. Strongly identified partisans (who tend to be affectively polarized) exhibit more alignment than weakly identified partisans38. Moreover, longitudinal research demonstrated that ideological consistency at time 1 predicted affective polarization at time 2, and affective polarization at time 1 predicted ideological consistency at time 2, all other things being equal82,87.

Cho, W. K. T., Gimpel, J. G. & Hui, I. S. Voter migration and the geographic sorting of the American electorate. Ann. Assoc. Am. Geogr. 103, 856–870 (2013).

Bolsen, T., Druckman, J. N. & Cook, F. L. The influence of partisan motivated reasoning on public opinion. Polit. Behav. 36, 235–262 (2014).

Iyengar, S. & Hahn, K. S. Red media, blue media: evidence of ideological selectivity in media use. J. Commun. 59, 19–39 (2009).

Third, disentangling the myriad effects of competing social influences on political polarization will require careful attention to the cognitive and motivational mechanisms that mediate message reception. It is usual to offer psychological speculations, such as the supposition that partisan communication activates group-justifying motives or that conservative communication activates system-justifying motives — and we have engaged in such speculation here. However, few (if any) studies actually connect the dots (see, for example, critiques of existing research on motivated political reasoning103,300,301). This is a critical step for drawing definitive conclusions about how specific independent variables influence the three types of political polarization discussed here.

Park, B. How are we apart? Continuity and change in the structure of ideological disagreement in the American public, 1980–2012. Soc. Forces 96, 1757–1784 (2018). This article describes three types of ideological disagreement — polarization, partisan sorting and dimensional alignment — and analyses historical trends in US public opinion for each dimension.

Carpenter, C. J. Cognitive dissonance, ego-involvement, and motivated reasoning. Ann. Int. Comun. Assoc. 43, 1–23 (2019).

McCarty, N., Poole, K. T. & Rosenthal, H. Polarized America: the Dance of Ideology and Unequal Riches (MIT Press, 2016).

In summary, ego, group and system justification mechanisms contribute to ideological and issue polarization, partisan alignment and affective polarization. Those who endorse liberal and conservative attitudes exhibit ego- and group-justifying biases favouring themselves and the groups to which they belong, contributing to symmetric forms of polarization99,140. However, they differ in terms of system-justifying (versus system-challenging) motives, which might help to explain asymmetric polarization10,34,47,48,73,74.

Stewart, A., McCarty, N. & Bryson, J. Polarization under rising inequality and economic decline. Sci. Adv. 6, eabd4201 (2020).

McConnell, C., Margalit, Y., Malhotra, N. & Levendusky, M. The economic consequences of partisanship in a polarized era. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 62, 5–18 (2017).

Thomsen, D. M. Opting Out Of Congress: Partisan Polarization And The Decline Of Moderate Candidates (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2017).

Boutyline, A. & Willer, R. The social structure of political echo chambers: variation in ideological homophily in online networks. Polit. Psychol. 38, 551–569 (2017).

Layered on top of ego-justifying biases are group-justifying biases99,139,140. Processes such as self-categorization, in-group favouritism and out-group derogation contribute to political polarization by encouraging partisans to engage in various forms of motivated reasoning — including selective information exposure or avoidance and confirmation or disconfirmation biases — on behalf of their political party or ideological group3,39,91,94,141,142,143,144. For example, people may evaluate the same information more favourably when it supports (versus contradicts) the beliefs, opinions and values of one’s political in-group103,115,120. Other studies suggest that when people are given the opportunity to censor the political out-group, many take it, even when the comments made by out-group members are inoffensive145.

Morgeson, F. V. III, Sharma, P. N., Sharma, U. & Hult, G. T. M. Partisan bias and citizen satisfaction, confidence, and trust in the US Federal Government. Public Manag. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1080/14719037.2021.1945667 (2021).

Harel, T. O., Jameson, J. K. & Maoz, I. The normalization of hatred: Identity, affective polarization, and dehumanization on Facebook in the context of intractable political conflict. Soc. Media Soc. 6, 1–10 (2020).

In terms of ideological and issue-based differences, those who identify as more conservative and endorse more rightist attitudes place a higher value on tradition, social order and stability, and the maintenance of existing hierarchical social arrangements. By contrast, those who identify as more liberal and endorse more leftist attitudes place a higher value on equality and progressive forms of social change43,47,82,107,171,172,173. For example, analyses of historical trends in public opinion from 1972 to 2016 reveal that Democrats were early adopters of more egalitarian views about gender, civil rights and gay rights, as well as more progressive on moral issues, whereas Republicans resisted these secular trends and were slower to change their attitudes40. This, in turn, contributed to an asymmetric form of polarization on some issues, such that Democrats shifted more rapidly than Republicans174.

Huckfeldt, R., Mendez, J. M. & Osborn, T. Disagreement, ambivalence, and engagement: the political consequences of heterogeneous networks. Polit. Psychol. 25, 65–95 (2004).

Feygina, I., Jost, J. T. & Goldsmith, R. System justification, the denial of global warming, and the possibility of ‘system-sanctioned change’. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 326–338 (2010).

Groenendyk, E. & Krupnikov, Y. What motivates reasoning? A theory of goal-dependent political evaluation. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 65, 180–196 (2021).

Fridman, A., Gershon, R. & Gneezy, A. COVID-19 and vaccine hesitancy: a longitudinal study. PLoS ONE 16, e0250123 (2021).

Lauka, A., McCoy, J. & Firat, R. B. Mass partisan polarization: measuring a relational concept. Am. Behav. Sci. 62, 107–126 (2018).

Diamond, L., Drutman, L., Lindberg, T., Kalmoe, N. P. & Mason, L. Opinion: Americans increasingly believe violence is justified if the other side wins. Politico (1 October, 2020).

Closely related to the phenomenon of myside bias are ‘naive realism’ and the objectivity illusion, whereby individuals assume, in a self-serving manner, that they process information in a more rational, impartial and accurate way than others do120. This, in turn, can lead to a failure to recognize the ways in which one’s own thinking is distorted by non-rational influences (the ‘bias blind spot’121). Paradoxically, the objectivity illusion can lead people to underestimate the epistemic confidence of their adversaries, by assuming that their adversaries are more aware of their own biases than they actually may be122.

Mijs, J. J. The paradox of inequality: income inequality and belief in meritocracy go hand in hand. Socioecon. Rev. 19, 7–35 (2021).

A case in point is the cleavage between those who supported the Brexit referendum and those who opposed it in the UK. Both sides exhibited ego-justifying and group-justifying biases that increased issue polarization on economic matters335, and high levels of affective polarization ensued336. In Germany, opinions about whether to welcome asylum seekers became highly polarized, with opponents embracing system-justifying stereotypes and exhibiting motivated reasoning on the question of whether asylum seekers increase rates of criminal activity319. Similar patterns have been observed in the USA294. Anti-immigration attitudes were shaped by social-communicative processes, including elite rhetoric and partisan cue-taking337. All of this parallels polarization trends in other regions, including Europe338, the Middle East11, and Latin America11.

Ma, Y., Dixon, G. & Hmielowski, J. Psychological reactance from reading basic facts on climate change: the role of prior views and political identification. Environ. Commun. 13, 71–86 (2019).

Jost, J. T., Hennes, E. P. & Lavine, H. in The Oxford Handbook of Social Cognition (ed. Carlston, D. E.) 851–875 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2013).

Kingzette, J. et al. How affective polarization undermines support for democratic norms. Public Opin. Q. 85, 663–677 (2021).

Levendusky, M. Our Common Bonds: Using What Americans Share to Overcome the Partisan Divide (Univ. Chicago Press, 2023).

Hogg, M. A., Turner, J. C. & Davidson, B. Polarized norms and social frames of reference: a test of the self-categorization theory of group polarization. Basic. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 11, 77–100 (1990).

Iyengar, S., Lelkes, Y., Levendusky, M., Malhotra, N. & Westwood, S. J. The origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 22, 129–146 (2019).

Huber, G. A. & Malhotra, N. Political homophily in social relationships. J. Polit. 79, 269–283 (2017). Using an online experiment and observational data from an online dating community, this article shows that USA residents are more inclined to date individuals who have similar (versus dissimilar) political characteristics to themselves.

In reviewing the literature, we sidestep the questions of whether citizens ‘are’ or ‘are not’ polarized and whether the degree of polarization in society has increased, decreased or stayed the same. The answers to such questions invariably depend upon somewhat arbitrary criteria. For example, it is unclear how much polarization is ‘enough’ to reach some categorical threshold or what time frame should be used to investigate trends. Because of space constraints, we are unable to cover various structural and institutional factors62 — including demographic shifts63, party systems and electoral rules64, and economic performance and inequality65,66 — that may influence levels of elite and mass polarization over time. We focus primarily on the US context, because this is where the vast majority of the relevant research to date has been conducted. However, high levels of political conflict have been reported in other countries, especially in Europe12,60,65,67,68,69,70,71.

Goel, S., Mason, W. & Watts, D. J. Real and perceived attitude agreement in social networks. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 611–621 (2010).